Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives
Bering (2006) put forward the claim that the deceased are viewed as authoritative moral figures, and Bering, MacLeod, and Shackelford (2005) present evidence supporting this. We extend Bering's conjecture through a within-subjects quasi-experimental study testing the possibility that person per...
Saved in:
Main Authors: | , , , , |
---|---|
Other Authors: | |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
2017
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://hdl.handle.net/10356/86299 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/43990 |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
Institution: | Nanyang Technological University |
Language: | English |
id |
sg-ntu-dr.10356-86299 |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
sg-ntu-dr.10356-862992020-03-07T12:10:38Z Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives Galang, Adrianne John Real Ellescas, Bianca Ysabel C. Santos, Jan Marie E. Locsin, Maria Aisha V. Sy, Keena Mayumi D. School of Humanities and Social Sciences Traits Person perception Bering (2006) put forward the claim that the deceased are viewed as authoritative moral figures, and Bering, MacLeod, and Shackelford (2005) present evidence supporting this. We extend Bering's conjecture through a within-subjects quasi-experimental study testing the possibility that person perception regarding personality traits might shift in a context where (a) the target is someone known personally, and (b) the target happens to be deceased. One-hundred ten undergraduate students in Manila were asked to rate the Big Five traits of two older adult individuals known to them personally, one of whom must be alive, and the other deceased. Using multilevel modeling, we found that decedent targets on average were rated with higher Extraversion and Agreeableness scores compared to living targets, and that this held even when controlling for relational closeness and other possible covariates. Additionally, relational closeness was associated with higher ratings for all traits regardless of whether they were alive or not, indicating the possibility of a halo effect. Accepted version 2017-11-03T07:59:45Z 2019-12-06T16:19:57Z 2017-11-03T07:59:45Z 2019-12-06T16:19:57Z 2017 Journal Article Galang, A. J. R., Ellescas, B. Y. C., Santos, J. M. E., Locsin, M. A. V., & Sy, K. M. D. (2017). Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 57-60. 0191-8869 https://hdl.handle.net/10356/86299 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/43990 10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.051 en Personality and Individual Differences © 2017 Elsevier. This is the author created version of a work that has been peer reviewed and accepted for publication by Personality and Individual Differences, Elsevier. It incorporates referee’s comments but changes resulting from the publishing process, such as copyediting, structural formatting, may not be reflected in this document. The published version is available at: [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.051]. 12 p. application/pdf |
institution |
Nanyang Technological University |
building |
NTU Library |
country |
Singapore |
collection |
DR-NTU |
language |
English |
topic |
Traits Person perception |
spellingShingle |
Traits Person perception Galang, Adrianne John Real Ellescas, Bianca Ysabel C. Santos, Jan Marie E. Locsin, Maria Aisha V. Sy, Keena Mayumi D. Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
description |
Bering (2006) put forward the claim that the deceased are viewed as authoritative moral figures, and Bering, MacLeod, and Shackelford (2005) present evidence supporting this. We extend Bering's conjecture through a within-subjects quasi-experimental study testing the possibility that person perception regarding personality traits might shift in a context where (a) the target is someone known personally, and (b) the target happens to be deceased. One-hundred ten undergraduate students in Manila were asked to rate the Big Five traits of two older adult individuals known to them personally, one of whom must be alive, and the other deceased. Using multilevel modeling, we found that decedent targets on average were rated with higher Extraversion and Agreeableness scores compared to living targets, and that this held even when controlling for relational closeness and other possible covariates. Additionally, relational closeness was associated with higher ratings for all traits regardless of whether they were alive or not, indicating the possibility of a halo effect. |
author2 |
School of Humanities and Social Sciences |
author_facet |
School of Humanities and Social Sciences Galang, Adrianne John Real Ellescas, Bianca Ysabel C. Santos, Jan Marie E. Locsin, Maria Aisha V. Sy, Keena Mayumi D. |
format |
Article |
author |
Galang, Adrianne John Real Ellescas, Bianca Ysabel C. Santos, Jan Marie E. Locsin, Maria Aisha V. Sy, Keena Mayumi D. |
author_sort |
Galang, Adrianne John Real |
title |
Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
title_short |
Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
title_full |
Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
title_fullStr |
Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
title_full_unstemmed |
Systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
title_sort |
systematic bias in trait attributions for deceased friends and relatives |
publishDate |
2017 |
url |
https://hdl.handle.net/10356/86299 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/43990 |
_version_ |
1681047910285836288 |