Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly
We investigated the circumstances in which 3- to 5-year-old children can and cannot interpret adjectives flexibly. In Experiment 1, children were required to interpret big and little both in reference to a basic level kind (e.g., “This is a big marble”) and in reference to a superordinate kind (e.g....
Saved in:
Main Authors: | , , , |
---|---|
Other Authors: | |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
2014
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://hdl.handle.net/10356/99439 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/24051 |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
Institution: | Nanyang Technological University |
Language: | English |
id |
sg-ntu-dr.10356-99439 |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
sg-ntu-dr.10356-994392020-03-07T12:10:40Z Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly Gao, Helena Hong Zelazo, Philip David Sharpe, Dean Mashari, Azad School of Humanities and Social Sciences DRNTU::Humanities::Linguistics We investigated the circumstances in which 3- to 5-year-old children can and cannot interpret adjectives flexibly. In Experiment 1, children were required to interpret big and little both in reference to a basic level kind (e.g., “This is a big marble”) and in reference to a superordinate kind (e.g., “This is a little toy”). Experiment 2 examined 3-year-olds’ flexible interpretation of big and little with respect to a medium-sized stimulus that was alternately compared with a smaller stimulus and a larger stimulus (e.g., “Which one of these two circles is the big one?”). Even the youngest children switched between interpretations when the switch was accompanied by a change in the stimulus display. When the stimulus display remained constant, however, younger children typically perseverated on a single interpretation. Results replicate evidence of the roots of flexible adjective interpretation but also show protracted development of the ability to coordinate two incompatible interpretations of a single situation. Accepted Version 2014-10-17T01:38:50Z 2019-12-06T20:07:18Z 2014-10-17T01:38:50Z 2019-12-06T20:07:18Z 2014 2014 Journal Article Gao, H. H., Zelazo, P. D., Sharpe, D., & Mashari, A. (2014). Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly. Cognitive development, 32, 86-102. 0885-2014 https://hdl.handle.net/10356/99439 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/24051 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.08.003 182494 en Cognitive development © 2014 Elsevier Inc. This is the author created version of a work that has been peer reviewed and accepted for publication by Cognitive Development, Elsevier Inc. It incorporates referee’s comments but changes resulting from the publishing process, such as copyediting, structural formatting, may not be reflected in this document. The published version is available at: [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.08.003]. 17 p. application/pdf |
institution |
Nanyang Technological University |
building |
NTU Library |
country |
Singapore |
collection |
DR-NTU |
language |
English |
topic |
DRNTU::Humanities::Linguistics |
spellingShingle |
DRNTU::Humanities::Linguistics Gao, Helena Hong Zelazo, Philip David Sharpe, Dean Mashari, Azad Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
description |
We investigated the circumstances in which 3- to 5-year-old children can and cannot interpret adjectives flexibly. In Experiment 1, children were required to interpret big and little both in reference to a basic level kind (e.g., “This is a big marble”) and in reference to a superordinate kind (e.g., “This is a little toy”). Experiment 2 examined 3-year-olds’ flexible interpretation of big and little with respect to a medium-sized stimulus that was alternately compared with a smaller stimulus and a larger stimulus (e.g., “Which one of these two circles is the big one?”). Even the youngest children switched between interpretations when the switch was accompanied by a change in the stimulus display. When the stimulus display remained constant, however, younger children typically perseverated on a single interpretation. Results replicate evidence of the roots of flexible adjective interpretation but also show protracted development of the ability to coordinate two incompatible interpretations of a single situation. |
author2 |
School of Humanities and Social Sciences |
author_facet |
School of Humanities and Social Sciences Gao, Helena Hong Zelazo, Philip David Sharpe, Dean Mashari, Azad |
format |
Article |
author |
Gao, Helena Hong Zelazo, Philip David Sharpe, Dean Mashari, Azad |
author_sort |
Gao, Helena Hong |
title |
Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
title_short |
Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
title_full |
Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
title_fullStr |
Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
title_full_unstemmed |
Beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
title_sort |
beyond early linguistic competence : development of children's ability to interpret adjectives flexibly |
publishDate |
2014 |
url |
https://hdl.handle.net/10356/99439 http://hdl.handle.net/10220/24051 |
_version_ |
1681040120687362048 |